Home |
Search |
Today's Posts |
#71
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Roger Hayter wrote:
Stephen Thomas Cole wrote: Roger Hayter wrote: Stephen Thomas Cole wrote: Gents, can you please **** off. If you have nothing to contribute to the discussion why join in at all? One minute it's ok to have a joke on Usenet, the next it isn't. Make up your mind, Rog! Your inconsistency is making you look vindictive, OM. "Gents,can you please **** off" is just too subtle a joke for me, I didn't get it at first. Now you point it out it is uproariously funny. I don't know how I could have missed that! I find it troubling that, despite having such a monumentally defective humour gland, you've unilaterally declared yourself the final arbiter on what jokes are funny enough to pass moderation. You couldn't make it up! -- STC / M0TEY / http://twitter.com/ukradioamateur |
#72
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Roger Hayter wrote on 11/19/2017 4:45 AM:
rickman wrote: snip I have seen no reasonable explanation of how a reed switch is any more analog than a mechanical switch. They are both analog movements of a mechanism. The only difference is one adjusts a magnetic field while the other applies pressure to a spring which bends (again in an analog manner) until it begins to make contact with another spring. Which will have more jitter? Only a measurement will say and the reed switch has a much lower time of bouncing, so will not possibly have a noticeable jitter in time of actuation. I agree that jitter is not a major issue, but I do think that with a mechanical switch that human muscles and brain are directly part of the analogue feedback loop controlling jitter in a way they may not be with an intervening magnetic link. At least with a straight key, if springs are part ot the system then your argument is more convincing. This is a different issue from contact bounce, which occurs after the initial contact closure. Can you explain how the two differ? Just saying they are different isn't an explanation. Of course any key has to have a spring for returning the key to home position. Every key I've seen had at least two adjustments, home position (and therefore the travel to contact closure) and spring tension. Bounce time is still and issue because most debounce circuits use a simple RC to resolve the bounce time and this adds delay to the contact closure time as seen by the equipment. -- Rick C Viewed the eclipse at Wintercrest Farms, on the centerline of totality since 1998 |
#73
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
rickman wrote:
Roger Hayter wrote on 11/19/2017 4:45 AM: rickman wrote: snip I have seen no reasonable explanation of how a reed switch is any more analog than a mechanical switch. They are both analog movements of a mechanism. The only difference is one adjusts a magnetic field while the other applies pressure to a spring which bends (again in an analog manner) until it begins to make contact with another spring. Which will have more jitter? Only a measurement will say and the reed switch has a much lower time of bouncing, so will not possibly have a noticeable jitter in time of actuation. I agree that jitter is not a major issue, but I do think that with a mechanical switch that human muscles and brain are directly part of the analogue feedback loop controlling jitter in a way they may not be with an intervening magnetic link. At least with a straight key, if springs are part ot the system then your argument is more convincing. This is a different issue from contact bounce, which occurs after the initial contact closure. Can you explain how the two differ? Just saying they are different isn't an explanation. A straight key is a simple proportional muscle movement with feedback loop via the feel of the contact closure. A key that works via a spring has a phase change to to the effective energy storage (inductance analogue) of the spring. The brain is very good at correcting very slight innacuracies if there is such simple motor feedback circuit, musicians do it all the time. If there is a magnetic movement or an optical circuit then a complex delay is included which is no longer a very simple function of muscular position, Unless the magnetic reaction of the reed switch is very reproducible or the optical link is very fast. The human brain can probably have its otherwise very accurate judgement of cadence harmed by a very few milliseconds of non-linearity. I do agree it should be possible to arrange such circuits so they are too fast to notice, but they would have to be very fast. Of course any key has to have a spring for returning the key to home position. Every key I've seen had at least two adjustments, home position (and therefore the travel to contact closure) and spring tension. The spring in a straight key varies the force but this is linear with displacement and not out of phase as it is with a springy linkage. Bounce time is still and issue because most debounce circuits use a simple RC to resolve the bounce time and this adds delay to the contact closure time as seen by the equipment. Most debouncing circuits nowadays would be implemented by digital device ignoring input for a certain time rather than a simple RC circuit. Probably a microprocessor. -- Roger Hayter |
#74
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On 11/19/2017 12:55 PM, Stephen Thomas Cole wrote:
Jerry Stuckle wrote: On 11/19/2017 12:35 AM, Stephen Thomas Cole wrote: Reported to ISP for off-topic posting. ROFLMAO! As if you ever posted anything on topic! Stuff it, Cole. Reported to ISP for personal abuse. Yes, you should report yourself for personal abuse, Cole! -- ================== Remove the "x" from my email address Jerry, AI0K ================== |
#75
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On 11/19/2017 12:55 PM, Stephen Thomas Cole wrote:
Jerry Stuckle wrote: On 11/19/2017 12:35 AM, Stephen Thomas Cole wrote: Reported to ISP for off-topic posting. ROFLMAO! As if you ever posted anything on topic! Stuff it, Cole. Reported to ISP for personal abuse. Reported to ISP for off-topic posting. -- ================== Remove the "x" from my email address Jerry, AI0K ================== |
#76
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Roger Hayter wrote on 11/19/2017 6:27 PM:
rickman wrote: Roger Hayter wrote on 11/19/2017 4:45 AM: rickman wrote: snip I have seen no reasonable explanation of how a reed switch is any more analog than a mechanical switch. They are both analog movements of a mechanism. The only difference is one adjusts a magnetic field while the other applies pressure to a spring which bends (again in an analog manner) until it begins to make contact with another spring. Which will have more jitter? Only a measurement will say and the reed switch has a much lower time of bouncing, so will not possibly have a noticeable jitter in time of actuation. I agree that jitter is not a major issue, but I do think that with a mechanical switch that human muscles and brain are directly part of the analogue feedback loop controlling jitter in a way they may not be with an intervening magnetic link. At least with a straight key, if springs are part ot the system then your argument is more convincing. This is a different issue from contact bounce, which occurs after the initial contact closure. Can you explain how the two differ? Just saying they are different isn't an explanation. A straight key is a simple proportional muscle movement with feedback loop via the feel of the contact closure. A key that works via a spring has a phase change to to the effective energy storage (inductance analogue) of the spring. The brain is very good at correcting very slight innacuracies if there is such simple motor feedback circuit, musicians do it all the time. If there is a magnetic movement or an optical circuit then a complex delay is included which is no longer a very simple function of muscular position, Unless the magnetic reaction of the reed switch is very reproducible or the optical link is very fast. The human brain can probably have its otherwise very accurate judgement of cadence harmed by a very few milliseconds of non-linearity. I do agree it should be possible to arrange such circuits so they are too fast to notice, but they would have to be very fast. I have no idea what you are talking about with a "complex delay". The reed switch is a switch just like any other. The mechanical switch has contacts just like the reed switch. Trying to invoke the human body and nervous system doesn't explain the difference in the switches you are picturing. Why wouldn't the optical and reed switches be fast? I've seen optocouplers that are good for 10 MHz data rates. The reed switches I've seen are rated for multiple kHz operation. What would make you think either of these are too slow to provide the contact closure for a mechanical device??? How do you define "very fast"??? Everything I've seen that was mechanical in nature is *much* slower than electronic devices. Of course any key has to have a spring for returning the key to home position. Every key I've seen had at least two adjustments, home position (and therefore the travel to contact closure) and spring tension. The spring in a straight key varies the force but this is linear with displacement and not out of phase as it is with a springy linkage. Where is a "springy linkage"??? You must be picturing something very different from what I am picturing. Bounce time is still and issue because most debounce circuits use a simple RC to resolve the bounce time and this adds delay to the contact closure time as seen by the equipment. Most debouncing circuits nowadays would be implemented by digital device ignoring input for a certain time rather than a simple RC circuit. Probably a microprocessor. Debounce circuits are implemented by the people designing them. Who inserts a digital device between the key and the radio? Where have you seen such circuits for a code key? When I searched for such circuits for use with CW everything I found was RC delays. -- Rick C Viewed the eclipse at Wintercrest Farms, on the centerline of totality since 1998 |
#77
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On Mon, 21 Dec 2015 23:33:17 +0000, gareth wrote:
For those who make their own Morse Keys, what do you use for the contacts, for I have found the phosphor bronze pillar for the dot contact on my ersatz Vibroplex to be very noisy and scratchy? I'm fairly sure that I asked this question before, but it has been a habit of never put off till tomorrow what you can put off till the day after. I chose phosphor bronze because in the days of making one's own electronic organs, phosphor bronze was suggested as a suitable keying matreial for the keyboards. I hope you get an answer, Gareth, because you wouldn't believe how common this question is. |
Reply |
Thread Tools | Search this Thread |
Display Modes | |
|
|
![]() |
||||
Thread | Forum | |||
Coastal Morse Stations Return to Air for Anniversary of Death of Morse Code | Shortwave | |||
Computer-Read Morse (Was Morse Code Binary?) | Policy | |||
FA: $9.99 MICROCRAFT "MORSE-A-WORD" MORSE CODE READER-NR | Swap | |||
FA: $9.99 MICROCRAFT "MORSE-A-WORD" MORSE CODE READER-NR | Equipment | |||
FA: $9.99 MICROCRAFT "MORSE-A-WORD" MORSE CODE READER-NR | Equipment |